Cl. Ojakangas et Tj. Ebner, PURKINJE-CELL COMPLEX SPIKE ACTIVITY DURING VOLUNTARY MOTOR LEARNING - RELATIONSHIP TO KINEMATICS, Journal of neurophysiology, 72(6), 1994, pp. 2617-2630
1. We examined the relationship of cerebellar Purkinje cell discharge
to the scaling of kinematics during a voluntary motor learning paradig
m. The study focused on whether the occurrence of complex spike (CS) d
ischarge was associated with kinematic changes. Two primates (Macaca m
ulatta) were trained to move a cursor using a two-joint manipulandum o
ver a horizontal video screen from a start target to one of four targe
t boxes. The relationship between the cursor and the hand (gain) was c
hanged, requiring scaling of movement distance to complete the task. A
s previously described, when the novel gain was presented over 100-200
movement trials the animals adapted their movements by using a strate
gy of scaling the amplitude and velocity of the first phase of the mov
ement while keeping time to peak velocity constant. 2. The paradigm co
nsisted of four different phases. A control phase at a gain of 1.0 was
initially performed. The learning phase over the next 180-210 movemen
ts used one of four gains (0.6, 0.75, 1.5, or 2.0). Last, a testing ph
ase involved and 80% of 100 trials at the learned gain and 20% of the
trials at the control gain of 1.0. The distance control phase consiste
d of using a gain of 1.0 but having the animal move to targets placed
at the distance and direction the hand moved in the adapted state. 3.
Simple spikes (SSs) and CSs of 141 Purkinje cells recorded primarily i
n the intermediate and lateral regions of zones V and VI in three cere
bellar hemispheres from the two primates were recorded during the dist
ance control, control, learning, and testing phases. Some cells were r
ecorded in lobule VII and Crus I. CS activity increased during the lea
rning phase, as documented previously. The increase in CS discharge oc
curred before or during the first 200-300 ms of the movement. This is
the same time period in which the kinematic changes necessary for adap
tation to the novel gain occur. Of 141 Purkinje cells recorded during
the learning paradigm, 104 (74%) demonstrated significant increases in
CS firing rate during the learning-testing phase. Of these 104 cells,
82 had statistically significant SS modulation. 4. Movement trials wi
th CSs were separated from the trials without CSs. Aligning the kinema
tic and spike train data on movement onset, the average velocity profi
les were subtracted from each other and a strict statistical criterion
applied to test for the significance of any differences. Movement tri
als randomly sorted into two groups served as a control. The kinematic
and spike train data were also sorted into two groups but aligned on
the occurrence of CSs. A Monte Carlo-type procedure was used to simula
te CS times for the non-CS movement trials. Of the 104 Purkinje cells
with a CS response, in 81 (78%) velocity was significantly different d
uring the movements with CSs compared with the movements without CSs.
This was observed whether the movements were aligned on movement onset
or CS occurrence. 5. The CS-associated velocity changes were uni- or
bimodal in shape. Mean maximum velocity difference was comparable in m
agnitude whether aligned on movement onset or CS occurrence. However,
time of peak velocity difference was more closely associated with CS o
ccurrence than with movement onset. Likewise, the latency of the veloc
ity change was tightly coupled to CS occurrence. Of the 35 Purkinje ce
lls with significant bimodal CS-aligned differences during CS trials,
the mean latency of the first velocity peak preceded the CS response o
nset by 71 ms, whereas the mean latency of the second peak was 205 ms
after the CS response. 6. The absolute amplitude of the velocity chang
e in the CS trials was related to the feedback gain, with the smaller
gains (i.e., adaptation requires larger movement amplitudes) associate
d with larger velocity differences. A contingency analysis showed that
the relationship between the feedback gain of the learning series and
the velocity difference is a dependent one. When adapting to the high
gains, the CSs occur preferentially in movements with a larger veloci
ty. Furthermore, for those cells in which the CS occurrence is associa
ted with a decrease in velocity, the feedback gain had a greater proba
bility of being <1. This finding suggests that CSs are more likely to
occur in trials in which the velocity difference is inappropriate for
the feedback gain the animal is required to learn. 7. For the 81 Purki
nje cells with significant velocity changes during CS trials, SS modul
ation as a percentage of background firing rate was analyzed in relati
on to CS occurrence. All cells had a brief SS inactivation period afte
r the CS that lasted from 10 to 20 ms. The SS firing in the interval f
rom 20 to 100 ms after CS occurrence was compared with the background
SS firing for the 81 cells with velocity changes. In 61 Purkinje cells
, the SS activity did not differ significantly from background in the
period from 20 to 100 ms after CS occurrence. The SS activity increase
d significantly in 13 cells and decreased in 7 cells. Therefore, in th
e Purkinje cells with CS-related velocity changes, the most common fin
ding was no significant change in the SS discharge beyond the inactiva
tion period. 8. We conclude that the climbing fiber system is involved
in the scaling of the movement kinematics necessary for adaptation du
ring this voluntary motor learning task. As shown previously, the mech
anism is not that predicted by the Marr-Albus hypothesis in which CS i
ncreases would be associated with long-term changes in SS activity. Ra
ther, the present results suggest that the climbing fiber system funct
ions either independently or by evoking shortterm changes in SS activi
ty to modify the open-loop phase of the movements. Because the CSs ten
d to occur in trials in which the velocity is inappropriate for the ga
in to be learned, we postulate that CSs are coupled to a velocity-rela
ted error signal.