The degree to which genetic factors influence human intelligence remai
ns a matter of some controversy. However, there is little doubt that s
ingle gene mutations can significantly alter brain development and fun
ction. For example, mutations affecting the FMR1 gene cause the fragil
e X syndrome, the most prevalent known inherited cause of intellectual
dysfunction. The most common mutation occurring in the FMR1 locus inv
olves expansion of a trinucleotide (CGG), repeat sequence within the p
romoter region of the gene(1). Between 6 and 54 repeats are typically
observed in individuals from the general population(2). When greater t
han or equal to 200 CGG repeats are present, the expanded repeat seque
nce and an adjacent CpG island are usually hypermethylated, a phenomen
on associated with transcriptional silencing of the gene and commonly
referred to as the FMR1 full mutation(3-5). The intermediate range of
repeats (approximately 50 to 200 CGGs), referred to as the premutation
, is characterized by the absence of hypermethylation within the promo
ter region and normal phenotype(6,7). Some individuals have a combinat
ion of a methylated and unmethylated alleles of differing size and are
referred to as having mosaic status. Most males with the FMR1 full mu
tation function in the mentally retarded range of intelligence; in con
trast, females with the FMR1 full mutation show a broader range of int
elligence, from mental retardation to normal IQ(8). The wider spectrum
of cognitive functioning in females with the full mutation is likely
to be due, in part, to variation in cellular X chromosome inactivation
patterns and consequent FMR1 protein (FMRP) production in the brain.
Despite differences in severity of intellectual dysfunction, both male
s and females with the FMR1 full mutation manifest a similar cognitive
profile with weakness in the visual-spatial and attentional-organizat
ional domains and relatively preserved verbal abilities(9,10). Here, w
e show that the variance in child IQ predicted by mean parental IQ is
decreased in 29 girls with the FMR1 full mutation compared to a non-fr
agile X group of 50 girls of similar age. We also show that activation
status of the FMR1 gene, but not repeat size, is strongly correlated
with intellectual function in girls with the full mutation, particular
ly for cognitive measures which distinguish fragile X from non-fragile
X groups. These data indicate that FMR1 activation status is directly
associated with the severity of intellectual dysfunction in girls wit
h the fragile X syndrome.