All visual art must obey the laws of the visual system. The first law
is that an image of the visual world is not impressed upon the retina,
but assembled together in the visual cortex. Consequently, many of th
e visual phenomena traditionally attributed to the eye actually occur
in the cortex. Among these is visual motion. The second law is that of
the functional specialization of the visual cortex, by which we mean
that separate attributes of the visual scene are processed in geograph
ically separate parts of the visual cortex, before being combined to g
ive a unified and coherent picture of the visual world. The third law
is that the attributes that are separated, and separately processed, i
n the cerebral cortex are those which have primacy in vision, These ar
e colour, form, motion and, possibly, depth. It follows that motion is
an autonomous visual attribute, separately processed and therefore ca
pable of being separately compromised after brain lesions. It is also
one of the visual attributes that have primacy, just like form or colo
ur or depth. We conclude that it is this separate visual attribute whi
ch those involved in kinetic art have tried to exploit, instinctively
and physiologically, from which it follows that in their explorations
artists are unknowingly exploring the organization of the visual brain
though with techniques unique to them.