What is the input to the mental system that computes inflected forms l
ike walked, came, dogs, and men? Recent connectionist models feed a wo
rd's phonological features into a single network, allowing it to gener
alize both regular and irregular phonological patterns, like stop-stop
ped, step-stepped and fling-flung, cling-clung. But for adults, phonol
ogical input is insufficient: verbs derived from nouns like ring the c
ity always have regular past tense forms (ringed), even if they are ph
onologically identical to irregular verbs (ring the bell. Similarly, n
ouns based on names, like two Mickey Mouses, and compounds based on po
ssessing rather than being their root morpheme, such as two saber-toot
hs, take regular plurals, even when they are homophonous with irregula
r nouns like mice and teeth. In four experiments, testing 70 three- to
ten-year-old children, we found that children are sensitive to such n
onphonological information: they were more likely to produce regular i
nflected forms for forms like to ring ('to put a ring on') and snaggle
tooth (a kind of animal doll with big teeth) than for their homophonou
s irregular counterparts, even when these counterparts were also exten
ded in meaning. Children's inflectional systems thus seem to be like a
dults': irregular forms are tied, to the lexicon but regular forms are
computed by a default rule, and words are represented as morphologica
l tree structures reflecting their derivation from basic-words are roo
ts. Such structures, which determine how novel complex words are deriv
ed and interpreted, also govern whether words with irregular sound pat
terns will be regularized: a word can be irregular only if its structu
re contains an irregular root in, 'head' position, allowing the lexica
lly stored irregular information to percolate up to apply to the word
as a whole. In all other cases, the inflected form is computed by a de
fault regular rule. This proposal fits the facts better than alternati
ves' appealing to ambiguity reduction or semantic similarity to a word
's central sense. The results, together with an analysis of adult spee
ch to children, suggest that morphological structure and a distinction
between mechanisms for regular and irregular inflection may be inhere
nt to the design of children's language systems.