In acquiring language, babies learn not only that people can communicate ab
out objects and events, but also that they typically use a particular kind
of act as the communicative signal. The current studies asked whether 1-yea
r-olds' learning of names during joint attention is guided by the expectati
on that names will be in the form of spoken words. In the first study, 13-m
onth-olds were introduced to either a novel word or a novel sound-producing
action (using a small noisemaker). Both the word and the sound were produc
ed by a re searcher as she showed the baby a new toy during a joint attenti
on episode. The baby's memory for the link between the word or sound and th
e object was tested in a multiple choice procedure. Thirteen-month-olds lea
rned both the word-object and sound-object correspondences, as evidenced by
their choosing the target reliably in response to hearing the word or soun
d on test trials, but not on control trials when no word or sound was prese
nt. In the second study, 13-month-olds, but not 20-month-olds, learned a ne
w sound-object correspondence. These results indicate that infants initiall
y accept a broad range of signals in communicative contexts and narrow the
range with development.