The English auxiliary have may reduce to the nonsyllabic form /v/ in c
ertain environments. This phenomenon (known as have contraction) has b
een adopted by proponents of derivational approaches to syntax (e.g. R
adford 1988: 414) as evidence for the existence of traces and of verb
movement in general. However, closer examination of the data reveals t
hat have contraction only occurs when three conditions apply. i. when
the auxiliary is finite; ii. when it follows a pronoun with an empty c
oda, iii. when that pronoun is the whole subject, that is, is not conj
oined with any other noun or pronoun. It is not clear how the movement
approach can account for conditions (ii) and (iii) above. My proposal
is that the frequent occurrence of strings conforming to the above co
nditions is such that over time the auxiliary verb has become an affix
. The solution is offered within the framework of lexical-functional g
rammar, a unification-based approach in which words and phrases are re
presented at copresent parallel levels containing functional and categ
orial information (S-structure and c-structure). The two individual-st
ructures created by the pronoun and the affix are able to unify to cre
ate the larger f-structure of an inflected pronoun. The affix v attach
es to its pronominal host through morphological composition using insi
de-out function application. In this way the affix is able to supply i
nformation regarding TENSE and ASP to the whole sentence. This is bloc
ked if the inflected pronoun is part of a conjoined subject, explainin
g the ungrammaticality of sentences with conjoined NP subjects, one of
which is an inflected pronoun This behavior would not be predicted by
theories that assume that have contraction is a purely surface (phras
e-structure) phonetic phenomenon.