Second-order reversed phi

Citation
Zl. Lu et G. Sperling, Second-order reversed phi, PERC PSYCH, 61(6), 1999, pp. 1075-1088
Citations number
50
Categorie Soggetti
Psycology
Journal title
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
ISSN journal
00315117 → ACNP
Volume
61
Issue
6
Year of publication
1999
Pages
1075 - 1088
Database
ISI
SICI code
0031-5117(199908)61:6<1075:SRP>2.0.ZU;2-5
Abstract
In a first-order reversed-phi motion stimulus (Anstis, 1970), the black-whi te contrast of successive frames is reversed, and the direction of apparent motion may, under some conditions, appear to be reversed. It is demonstrat ed here that, for many classes of stimuli, this reversal is a mathematical property of the stimuli themselves, and the real problem is in perceiving f orward motion, which involves the second- or third-order motion systems or both. Three classes of novel second-order reversed-phi stimuli (contrast, s patial frequency, and flicker modulation) that are invisible to first-order motion analysis were constructed. In these stimuli, the salient stimulus f eatures move in the forward (feature displacement) direction, but the secon d-order motion energy model predicts motion in the reversed direction. In p eripheral vision, for all stimulus types and all temporal frequencies, all the observers saw only the reversed-phi direction of motion. In central vis ion, the observers also perceived reversed motion at temporal frequencies a bove about 4 Hz, but they perceived movement in the forward direction at lo wer temporal frequencies. Since all of these stimuli are invisible to first -order motion, these results indicate that the second-order reversed-phi st imuli activate two subsequent competing motion mechanisms, both of which in volve an initial stage of texture grabbing (spatiotemporal filtering, follo wed by fullwave rectification). The second-order motion system then applies a Reichardt detector (or equivalently, motion energy analysis) directly to this signal and arrives at the reversed-phi direction. The third-order sys tem marks the location of features that differ from the background (the fig ure) in a salience map and computes motion in the forward direction from th e changes in the spatiotemporal location of these marks. The second-order s ystem's report of reversed movement dominates in peripheral vision and in c entral vision at higher temporal frequencies, because it has better spatial and temporal resolution than the third-order system, which has a cutoff fr equency of 3-4 Hz (Lu & Sperling, 1995b). In central vision, below 3-4 Hz, the third-order system's report of resolvable forward movement of something salient (the figure) dominates the second-order system's report of texture contrast movement.