David Lack proposed that parental feeding ability ultimately limited clutch
size in bird species in which the young were dependent upon their parents
for food. However, many species can raise broods that are larger than their
normal clutch size. Based on nine years of experimental results from an in
dividually marked population of Eastern Kingbirds (Tyrannus tyrannus) breed
ing in central New York (USA), I test six hypotheses that have been propose
d as explanations for why birds fail to lay larger, seemingly more producti
ve clutch sizes. I modified brood sizes by adding or removing 1-2 nestlings
when broods were 1-3 d old and then documented the effects of brood size a
nd manipulated brood size on nestling size and survivorship, offspring recr
uitment, adult survival, and future adult reproduction. Most first clutches
of the season held three eggs (62% of 503 clutches), but the proportion of
young to hedge did not vary with brood size (1-5 young), and as a result,
broods of five were the most productive. Lack's basic food-limitation model
was thus rejected. Although nestling mass and ninth-primary length at fled
ging declined with brood size, offspring survival during the immediate 10-1
2 d period after fledging was unrelated to nestling mass or lengths of the
tarsus or ninth primary. The findings that the underweight young in broods
of four and five did not suffer disproportionate mortality and that they we
re just as likely to appear as recruits in future years led to a rejection
of the extended version of Lack's food-limitation model.
Comparisons of annual variation in the relationship between productivity an
d brood size showed that productivity increased with brood size in eight of
nine years (significant in six years). Thus, high temporal stochastic vari
ation in conditions for rearing young (the "bad-years" hypothesis) is unlik
ely to explain the relatively small clutch size of kingbirds. Predictions o
f two other hypotheses that predict asymmetrically low survivorship of youn
g in large broods (the "cliff-edge" and "brood-parasitism" hypotheses) were
also rejected. On the other hand, evidence suggested that females individu
alize clutch size such that each female lays a clutch that matches her indi
vidual feeding ability.
Although fledgling production was not adversely affected by experimental in
creases in brood size, most enlarged broods lost young during the 10-12 d i
mmediately after fledging. Thus, enlarged broods ultimately produced no mor
e independent young than did control broods that began with the same number
of eggs, Fledgling deaths were not related to nestling mass or size, and r
ecruitment was independent of manipulations. Survival and fecundity costs o
f reproduction also existed for females. Male survival (68%) was independen
t of the number of young that had been raised (0-5 young), and future breed
ing efforts were not compromised by elevated effort in the past year. Howev
er, females that raised broods of five were less likely to return to breed
in the following year (42%) than were females that raised 2-4 young (62%).
Among the survivors, females that raised enlarged broods in the preceding y
ear also experienced more hatching failure and fledged fewer young than fem
ales that raised reduced broods in the preceding year. I suggest that costs
of reproduction probably set the ultimate limit to clutch size in Eastern
Kingbirds. I did not test the hypothesis that high rates of nest predation
favor the evolution of small clutch size, but given that predators destroye
d similar to 50% of nests each year, it is also likely that nest predation
has contributed to the evolution of the current clutch size of kingbirds. W
hether a female produces a clutch of three or four eggs is probably determi
ned by individual differences in parental ability, which may be related to
either intrinsic properties of the female or territory quality.