An intelligibility of over 90% was reported for keywords in "everyday" 1/3-
octave sentences centered on 1500 Hz and having steep transition band slope
s of 100 dB/octave [Warren et al., Percept. Psychophys. 57, 175-182 (1995)]
. A subsequent study by Warren and Bashford [J. Acoust. Sec. Am. 106, L47-L
52 (1999)] found that it was not the 1/3-octave passband, but the transitio
n bands that were chiefly responsible for this high intelligibility: When t
he passband and transition bands were segregated using filter slopes of 100
0 dB/octave, the isolated passband had an intelligibility score of only 24%
, while the pair of transition bands had a score of over 80%. In the presen
t study, experiment 1 examined the distribution of information along the tr
ansition bands' slopes by truncation at graded downpoints: Truncation at do
wnpoints of 40 dB or more produced no significant change in intelligibility
. Experiment 2 closed the gap separating the transition bands so that their
slopes intersected at 1500 Hz. This triangular band had a negligible passb
and (as defined conventionally by 3-dB downpoints) and an intelligibility s
core of 60%; truncation at downpoints of 50 dB or more produced no signific
ant change in intelligibility. Experiment 3 determined the intelligibilitie
s of rectangular bands (1000-dB/octave slopes) centered on 1500 Hz. Their b
andwidths ranged from 3 to 12 semitones In 1-semitone steps, resulting in i
ntelligibility scores increasing monotonically from 14% to 94%. Calculation
s based upon experiments 2 and 3 showed that the triangular band truncated
at 30-dB downpoints had half the intelligibility of a rectangular band havi
ng the same frequency range. (C) 2000 Acoustical Society of America. [S0001
-4966(00)01009-2].