Attraction of a predator to chemical information related to nonprey: when can it be adaptive?

Citation
T. Shimoda et M. Dicke, Attraction of a predator to chemical information related to nonprey: when can it be adaptive?, BEH ECOLOGY, 11(6), 2000, pp. 606-613
Citations number
55
Categorie Soggetti
Animal Sciences","Neurosciences & Behavoir
Journal title
BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY
ISSN journal
10452249 → ACNP
Volume
11
Issue
6
Year of publication
2000
Pages
606 - 613
Database
ISI
SICI code
1045-2249(200011/12)11:6<606:AOAPTC>2.0.ZU;2-Y
Abstract
Information specificity can be important to animals in making optimal decis ions. However, it is not always necessary to use every level of specificity . We analyzed the response of the predatory mite Phytoseiulus persimilis to plant-produced information related to a nonprey herbivore. This predator i s a specialist feeding on spider mites in the genus Tetranychus. Caterpilla rs of Spodoptera exigua cannot serve as prey. Plants respond to an infestat ion by herbivores with the emission of volatiles that attract carnivorous e nemies of the herbivores. Conspecific plants infested with different herbiv ore species can emit blends that are qualitatively identical, while differi ng in the ratios of blend components. However, different plant species emit volatile blends that differ qualitatively. We demonstrated that the predat or II persimilis is attracted to volatiles from bean plants infested with S . exigua caterpillars, but that this attraction is affected by predator sta rvation and host-plant experience. One-hour and 24-h starved predators were made to represent predators that just lost a prey patch versus predators t hat have totally lost a prey patch. Predators reared on spider mites on bea n were attracted to bean plants infested with caterpillars when starved for 1 h but not when starved for 24-h. Both predator groups were attracted to bean plants infested with prey (i.e., spider mites). One-hour starved preda tors can use the odor to relocate the rewarding prey patch they just lost c ontact with, and using a general olfactory representation of the blend is s ufficient for relocation. In contrast, for 24-h starved predators, the perc eption of a plant's odor blend is unlikely to represent the prey patch lost , and discriminating between an odor blend representing prey or nonprey wil l avoid investing time in finding a nonprey herbivore. In contrast, predato rs that had been reared on spider mites on cucumber and thus had experience d a qualitatively different odor blend were not attracted to volatiles from caterpillar-infested bean plants. They were attracted to spider mite-infes ted bean plants, irrespective of starvation level. To cucumber-experienced predators, the perception of bean plant odor cannot represent the prey patc h lost, but only a new prey patch. Being discriminative and only responding to prey-infested plants is adaptive in this situation. Our results are dis cussed in the context of optimal information processing.