Background: Developmental screening tests, even those meeting standards for
screening test accuracy, produce numerous false-positive results for 15% t
o 30% of children. This is thought to produce unnecessary referrals for dia
gnostic testing or special services and increase the cost of screening prog
rams.
Objectives: To explore whether children who pass screening tests differ in
important ways from those who do not and to determine whether children over
referred for testing benefit from the scrutiny of diagnostic testing and tr
eatment planning.
Methods: Subjects were a national sample of 512 parents and their children
(age range of the children, 7 months to 8 years) who participated in valida
tion studies of various screening tests. Psychological examiners adhering t
o standardized directions obtained informed consent and administered at lea
st 2 developmental screening measures (the Brigance Screens, the Battelle D
evelopmental Inventory Screening Test, the Denver-II, and the Parents' Eval
uations of Developmental Status) and a concurrent battery of diagnostic mea
sures, including tests of intelligence, language, and academic achievement
(for children aged 2 1/2 years and older). The performance on diagnostic me
asures of children who failed screening but were not found to have a disabi
lity (false positives) was compared with that of children who passed screen
ing and did not have a disability on diagnostic testing (true negatives).
Results: Children with false-positive scores performed significantly (P<.00
1) lower on diagnostic measures than did children with true-negative scores
. The false-positive group had scores in adaptive behavior, language, intel
ligence, and academic achievement that were 9 to 14 points lower than the s
cores of those in the true-negative group. When viewing the likelihood of s
coring below the 25th percentile on diagnostic measures, children with fals
e-positive scores had a relative risk of 2.6 in adaptive behavior (95% conf
idence interval [CI], 1.67-4.21), 3.1 in language skills (95% CI, 1.90-5.20
), 6.7 on intelligence tests (95% CI, 3.28-13.50), and 4.9 on academic meas
ures (95% CI, 2.61-9.28). Overall, 151 (70%) of the children with false-pos
itive results scored below the 25th percentile on 1 or more diagnostic meas
ures (the point at which most children have difficulty benefiting from typi
cal classroom instruction) in contrast with 64 (29%) of the children with t
rue-negative scores (odds ratio, 5.6; 95% CT, 3.73-8.49). Children with fal
se-positive scores were also more likely to be nonwhite and to have parents
who had not graduated from high school. Performance differences between ch
ildren with true-negative scores and children with false-positive scores co
ntinued to be significant (P<.001) even after adjusting for sociodemographi
c differences between groups.
Conclusions: Children overreferred for diagnostic testing by developmental
screens perform substantially lower than children with true-negative scores
on measures of intelligence, language, and academic achievement-the 3 best
predictors of school success. These children also carry more psychosocial
risk factors, such as limited parental education and minority status. Thus,
children with false-positive screening results are an at-risk group for wh
om diagnostic testing may not be an unnecessary expense but rather a benefi
cial and needed service that can help focus intervention efforts. Although
such testing will not indicate a need for special education placement, it c
an be useful in identifying children's needs for other programs known to im
prove language, cognitive, and academic skills, such as Head Start, Title I
services, tutoring, private speech-language therapy, and quality day care.